国产精品

If more people told you something was true, you鈥檇 think you would tend to believe it.

Not according to a by Yale University which found that people believe a single source of information which is repeated across many channels (a 鈥榝alse consensus鈥), just as readily as multiple people telling them something based on many independent original sources (a 鈥榯rue consensus鈥).

The finding showed how misinformation can be bolstered, and it had ramifications for important decisions we make based on advice we receive from places such as governments and media on information like , wearing masks during the pandemic, or even who we vote for in an election.

The 2019 鈥榠llusion of consensus鈥 finding has fascinated postdoctoral research associate in 国产精品 Science鈥檚 School of Psychology, Saoirse Connor Desai, who has tested the illusion finding and found a way for people to not be tricked with so-called 鈥榝ake news鈥 from a single source.

Her team鈥檚 study has been published in .

鈥淲e found that illusion can be reduced when we give people information about how the original sources used evidence to arrive at their conclusions,鈥 Dr Connor Desai says.

Saoirse Connor Desai

Dr Saoirse Connor Desai, postdoctoral research associate in 国产精品 Science鈥檚 School of Psychology

She says the finding is particularly relevant for science communication best practice 鈥 e.g. how policy makers or media present people with expert scientific evidence or research.

For instance, over 80 per cent of repeat claims from a single person who claims to be a 鈥榩olar bear expert鈥 .

鈥淵ou could have a situation where a misleading health proposal is repeated through multiple channels, which could influence people to rely on that information more than they should do, because they think there is evidence for that, or they think there is a consensus,鈥 Dr Connor Desai says.

鈥淏ut our finding shows that if you can explain to people where your information comes from, and how the original sources reached their conclusions, that strengthens their ability to identify a 鈥榯rue consensus鈥.鈥

Dr Connor Desai says the Yale study finding was surprising to her, 鈥渂ecause it seemed to be an indictment of human ability to distinguish between true consensus and false consensus鈥.

鈥淭he original study showed that people are routinely bad at this. There were lots of situations where they are never able to tell the difference between true and false consensus,鈥 she says.

鈥淚t鈥檚 problematic because if people hear a single person鈥檚 false or misleading statements repeated through different channels, they might feel the statement is more valid than it is.鈥

Dr Connor Desai says an example of this is multiple independent experts agreeing that (true consensus), versus a single group or individual saying that people should use it as an anti-viral drug (false consensus).

How the study was conducted

The aim of the 国产精品 study was to understand why people believe false information when it鈥檚 repeated. 聽

鈥淥ur main goal was to establish whether one reason that people are equally convinced by true and false consensus is that they assume that different sources share data or methodologies,鈥 Dr Connor Desai says.

鈥淒o they understand there is potentially more evidence when you have multiple experts saying the same thing?鈥

The 国产精品 researchers conducted several experiments.

The first experiment replicated the 2019 Yale study, which saw participants given a variety of articles about a fictional tax policy which took positive, negative, or neutral stances, and then asked to what extent they agreed the proposal would improve the economy.

It replicated the 鈥渋llusion of consensus鈥 where people are equally convinced by one piece of evidence as they are by many pieces of evidence but added a new condition where they told people who saw a 鈥渢rue consensus鈥 that the sources had used different data and methods to arrive at their conclusions.

The result was a reduction in the illusion of consensus.

鈥淧eople were more convinced by true consensus than false consensus.鈥

In another experiment, 200 participants were given information about an election in a fictional foreign democratic country.

They were shown fictional Twitter posts from news outlets that said which candidates would get more votes in the election: some sourced the same or different pollsters to predict a candidate would win, while another tweet said a different contender would win.

An example of the fake tweets used in the misinformation study

An example of the made up Twitter posts used in the study. Image: Supplied.

But in the true consensus Twitter posts, they gave people a scenario in which it was clear that different primary sources worked independently and used different data to arrive at their conclusions.

鈥淲e expected that many people would be more familiar with such polls and would realise that looking at multiple different polls would be a better way of predicting the election result than just seeing a single poll multiple times,鈥 Dr Connor Desai says.

After reading the tweets, participants rated which candidate would win based on the polling predictions.

鈥淚t seemed that people were more convinced by a true consensus than a false consensus when they understood the pollsters had gathered evidence independently of one another,鈥 Professor of Cognitive Psychology in the School of Psychology, Brett Hayes says.

鈥淥ur results suggest that people do see claims endorsed by multiple sources as stronger when they believe these sources really are independent of one another.鈥

The researchers later replicated and extended the tweet study with 365 more participants.

鈥淭his time we had a condition where the tweets came from individual people who showed their endorsement of the polls using emojis,鈥 Dr Connor Desai says.

鈥淩egardless of whether the tweets came from news outlets, or individual tweeters, people were more convinced by true than false consensus when the relationship between sources was unambiguous.鈥

False consensus not completely discounted

But the researchers also found the participants didn鈥檛 completely discount false consensus.

鈥淭here are at least two possible explanations for this effect,鈥 Dr Connor Desai said.

鈥淭he first is that such repetition simply increases the familiarity of the claim 鈥撀爀nhancing its memory representation, and this is sufficient to increase confidence.

鈥淭he second is that people may make inferences about why a claim is repeated because the source believed it was the most reputable or provided the strongest evidence.

鈥淔or instance, if different news channels all cite the same expert you might think that they鈥檙e citing the same person because they are the most qualified to talk about whatever it is they鈥檙e talking about.鈥

Dr Connor Desai plans to next look at why some communication strategies are more effective than others, and if repeating information is always effective.

鈥淚s there a point where there鈥檚 too much consensus, and people become suspicious?鈥 she says.

鈥淐an you correct a 鈥榝alse consensus鈥 by pointing out that it鈥檚 often better to get information from multiple independent sources? These are the kinds of strategies we wish to look into.鈥

.